Carhart-Harris, R. L., Wagner, A. C., Agrawal, M., Kettner, H., Rosenbaum, J. F., Gazzaley, A., Nutt, D. J., & Erritzoe, D. “Can Pragmatic Research, Real-World Data, and Digital Technologies Aid the Development of Psychedelic Medicine?” Journal of Psychopharmacology, Jan 2022.

Authors

Robin L Carhart-Harris, Anne C Wagner, Manish Agrawal, Hannes Kettner, Jerold F Rosenbaum, Adam Gazzaley, David J Nutt, and David Erritzoe 

Originally published in the Journal of Psychopharmacology as Can pragmatic research, real-world data and digital technologies aid the development of psychedelic medicine?

Published Date: January 2022

Abstract

Favourable regulatory assessments, liberal policy changes, new research centres and substantial commercial investment signal that psychedelic therapy is making a major comeback. Positive findings from modern trials are catalysing developments, but it is questionable whether current confirmatory trials are sufficient for advancing our understanding of safety and best practice. Here we suggest supplementing traditional confirmatory trials with pragmatic trials, real-world data initiatives and digital health solutions to better support the discovery of optimal and personalised treatment protocols and parameters. These recommendations are intended to help support the development of safe, effective and cost-efficient psychedelic therapy, which, given its history, is vulnerable to excesses of hype and regulation.

Keywords

Real-world data, pragmatic trials, psychedelics, serotonin


1Centre for Psychedelic Research, Imperial College London, London, UK

2Remedy, Toronto, Canada

3Department of Psychology, Ryerson University, Toronto, Canada

4Maryland Oncology and Hematology, Rockville, USA

5Aquilino Cancer Center, Rockville, USA

6Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, USA

7Neuroscape, Department of Neurology, Physiology and Psychiatry, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, USA

Corresponding author(s)

Robin L Carhart-Harris, Centre for Psychedelic Research, Imperial College London, Burlington Danes Building, London W12 0NN, UK. Email: r.carhart-harris@imperial.ac.uk

Close to one billion people are affected by mental illness and substance misuse worldwide. In many developed countries, mental illness ranks top for burden of disease (James et al., 2018), is more common, impactful and costly than other health conditions, and is a core component of overall health. The total cost of mental illness in the USA is estimated to be $2.5 trillion (Trautmann et al., 2016), the global antidepressant market is worth over $13.5 billion (Brandessence Market Research Company Pvt Ltd, 2019) and the wellness sector is estimated to be worth over $4.5 trillion (Global Wellness Institute, 2019).

Despite record increases in psychiatric medication prescription rates, the prevalence of mental illness is not reducing and may well be increasing in certain populations, such as the young (Twenge et al., 2019). There are indications that rates of mental illness have increased during the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic (Armitage, 2021; Inkster et al., 2020; Mahase, 2021). Evidence indicates that the efficacy of leading drug (Cipriani et al., 2018; Lewis et al., 2019) and psychological interventions (Flint et al., 2015) is modest, and there is scope for improved tolerability (Massabki and Abi-Jaoude, 2021) and access (Clark, 2011). Most mental health interventions are reactive. Effective prophylactic intervention would be hugely valuable (Patton et al., 2016). Relatedly, early life trauma (Varese et al., 2012) and mental illness (Copeland et al., 2009) are reliable predictors of future morbidity.

There is a legacy of division between the biological and psychological arms of mental health care and research. A notable initiative towards innovation in biomedical psychiatry is the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) (Insel and Cuthbert, 2015). The main principle of RDoC is that, since diagnostic criteria are a product of clinical expediency, transdiagnostically relevant pathological mechanisms and treatment targets may have been overlooked. Relatedly, there is now good evidence for genetic overlap between psychiatric disorders (Brainstorm et al., 2018). RDoC is primarily a biological initiative that aims to translate mental illness into ‘brain illness’, for the purpose of discovering candidate brain biomarkers and treatment targets (Insel and Cuthbert, 2015).

Notable initiatives towards innovation in psychological health care include efforts to improve the cost-effectiveness of (Richards et al., 2018), access to (Moller et al., 2019) and reach of psychotherapy – e.g. through utilising technological advances (Leff et al., 2014; Richards et al., 2018) and social and familial networks (Freeman et al., 2019). So-called ‘third wave’ psychotherapeutic approaches have gained traction, e.g. with a spike in the popularity of mindfulness (Segal and Teasdale, 2018) and growing interest in – and evidence for – acceptance and commitment therapy (Hayes, 2019). Bearing in mind relevance to RDoC, one important characteristic of these approaches is their alleged transdiagnostic relevance, i.e. that they seek to identify and target a common pathological mechanism, but more work is needed to link the relevant psychological constructs, such as ‘psychological flexibility’, with biological processes.

There are promising signs of confluence between psychiatry’s biological and psychological divisions however, including a growing appreciation of the value of both psychological and neurobiological accounts of mental illness and its aetiology, as well as how environment, mind, brain and body interface and interact (Carhart-Harris, 2018) – consistent with the ‘biopsychosocial’ model (Engel, 1977; Wade and Halligan, 2017). Specific examples of biopsychosocial research in psychiatry include studying: (1) gene × environment (Caspi et al., 2010) and (2) drug × environment interactions (Alboni et al., 2017) – of which drug-assisted psychotherapy can be considered an example (Carhart-Harris, 2018), (3) neurophenomenology (Varela, 1996) and (4) the biological mechanisms of psychological interventions (Brooks and Stein, 2015).

Into this arena comes psychedelic therapy, a quintessentially biopsychosocial intervention. Evidence indicates that psychedelic therapy is a particularly promising and progressive mental health care solution (Nutt et al., 2020). Classic serotonergic psychedelics can be most precisely defined by their pharmacology, i.e. agonist action at the serotonin 2A receptor, which, if blocked, effectively abolishes their signature psychological effects (Nichols, 2017). Psychedelic therapy is defined here as psychologically supported classic psychedelic drug experiences – although we recognise that psychotherapy alongside experiences induced by certain other psychoactive substances, e.g. MDMA and ketamine, bears relation to classic psychedelic therapy.

Psychedelic therapy has shown promise for a range of different mental health conditions, including: (1) depression (Carhart-Harris et al., 2016; Davis et al., 2020), (2) end-of-life anxiety (Griffiths et al., 2016), (3) addiction (Johnson et al., 2014) and (4) obsessive compulsive disorder (Moreno et al., 2006). Indirect evidence also supports its potential for treating eating disorders (Renelli et al., 2020; Spriggs et al., 2020) and chronic pain (Whelan and Johnson, 2018). See Andersen et al. (2020) for a review.

The Food and Drug Administration has granted ‘breakthrough therapy’ status to two independent multi-site double-blind randomised controlled trials (DB-RCTs), aiming to bring psilocybin therapy to marketing authorisation for depression, while related work is currently underway across Europe. Population (Hendricks et al., 2015) and controlled studies (Griffiths et al., 2018), as well as large retrospective (Forstmann et al., 2020) and prospective surveys (Haijen et al., 2018), are generating evidence for improved mental well-being across a large demographic, potentially opening psychedelic therapy up to a sizeable wellness market. The successful initiative to legalise psilocybin therapy in Oregon, USA, intentionally included access for healthy individuals.

In addition to its putative transdiagnostic utility, other reasons to feel optimistic about psychedelic therapy include: its novel action (Carhart-Harris and Friston, 2019), and rapid and enduring therapeutic impact (Nutt and Carhart-Harris, 2021; Nutt et al., 2020). Unlike traditional psychiatric drugs, positive effects have been observed for several months after just one or two doses. In terms of safety, psychedelics such as psilocybin (‘magic mushrooms’) have a favourable toxicity profile and therapeutic index, and negligible addiction potential (Rucker et al., 2018). Not wishing to neglect rare cases of putative iatrogenesis, including those of so-called ‘hallucinogen persisting perceptual disorder’ (Rucker et al., 2018), the main hazards of psilocybin therapy relate to the intensity of the psychological state produced by higher doses, and associated need for a carefully engineered contextual container, e.g. with effective psychological preparation, supervision and aftercare.

The utilisation of a drug-induced period of heightened cortical plasticity is likely to be a core component of psychedelic therapy’s mechanism of action (Carhart-Harris and Friston, 2019) and candidate functional (Carhart-Harris, 2018a) and anatomical biomarkers (Ly et al., 2018; Xing et al., 2020) of this are already being examined. In the context of a predictive processing framework, the ability of psychedelic therapy to (1) relax and (2) (with psychological support) recalibrate cognitive and behavioural biases may be a central part of its action (Carhart-Harris and Friston, 2019) – as may an accelerated learning rate (Harvey, 2003).

How can we best advance the science of psychedelic medicine? Here we advocate pragmatic considerations (Purgato et al., 2015), the utilisation of ‘basket’ protocols (Parmar et al., 2017), as well as digitally aided data registries. Distinguishing pragmatic from confirmatory trials, the former refers to the actual, realistic (‘real-world’) conditions under which a therapeutic intervention will be received (e.g. when approved and marketed), whereas confirmatory trials typically engineer experimental conditions to support strong scientific inferences, but these often poorly reflect real-world conditions. Basket protocols are defined as single protocols, approved by relevant research regulatory bodies, that allow for a single intervention to be tested for multiple different (but often linked) disorders or conditions, such as a single drug for different types of cancer.

Developers may be right to adhere to convention in delivering confirmatory trials, but, if resources and conditions allow, considerable benefits could be gained from more explorative study designs. Such exploration may be best served by research that can address a greater range of questions of clinical and real-world relevance. Similarly, whereas confirmatory trials may choose to constrain eligibility and treatment criteria, pragmatic trials may benefit from broadening them, while maintaining a sensibly high-bar for (assumed) contraindication-related exclusion criteria. Easy to sample biometrics (e.g. via wearable devices or sensors and simple electroencephalography) and behavioural sampling (e.g. natural language and phone-use) could accrue large pools of objective data with potential predictive value.

If such studies and data registries are designed with careful consideration of data quality and fitness, pragmatic research could create significant value for various different stakeholders, e.g. scientists, clinicians, regulators, health care systems, payers and investors. As has been the case with medical cannabis, treatment-seeking patients will look for guidance from clinicians who take theirs from scientific evidence. Liberal policy changes occurring prior to the conducting of sufficient research could create similar problems for clinicians as occurred with medical cannabis. Such imperfect clinical scenarios could, however, also represent opportunities for innovative pragmatic and observational research. The creation of electronic data registries, e.g. for prescribers of psilocybin therapy in regions legally permitting (regulated) access (such as Oregon, USA), may be one appealing example, enabling the collection of valuable real-world data.

Data registries and pragmatic trials will collect data from broad and diverse samples. Data on use among healthy individuals can be supplemented by data from individuals seeking psychedelic therapy treatment for depression, particularly as safety is being established in this population (Carhart-Harris et al., 2016; Davis et al., 2020). An even more ambitious project would be to utilise a protocol to only exclude individuals where there are good reasons to suspect elevated risk and inadequate specialist support. Indeed, future psychedelic therapy clinics in areas supporting legal access and/or operating under a (pragmatic) research mandate may support such a scenario. Moreover, utilising digital tools, such as cellphone apps (e.g. mydelica.com), to track outcomes linked to psychedelic use could generate large data pools that could be mined to inform on such matters as patient screening and treatment optimisation.

Whether via data registries annexed to legal-access psychedelic therapy or approved pragmatic research trials, or both, the proposed approaches can serve the agenda of identifying transdiagnostic treatment targets (Carhart-Harris and Friston, 2019). The RDoC initiative pays selective attention to phenotypes associated with pathology, neglecting parameters associated with wellness, and this may be an oversight. Evidence of (1) reliable and sustained improvements in well-being (Haijen et al., 2018) and lifestyle (Ona et al., 2019) with psychedelic therapy, as well as the maintenance of psychological wellness (Ross et al., 2016), (2) recognition of the bidirectional relationship between psychological and physical health, and (3) awareness of the substantial costs required to implement any human drug study, let alone a clinical trial with a psychedelic, and (4) combined with a need for greater safety data across a diverse demographic, particularly given the liberalising political climate surrounding psychedelics, are all good reasons to justify innovative and pragmatic approaches to researching psychedelic medicine.

Collecting large sample sizes will enable better prediction-of-response modelling (Haijen et al., 2018), which will help mitigate risk and inform the potential customisation of care. A multi-site ‘trial’ or centralised registry would help generate and store the large data needed for reliable prediction-of-response modelling, with the added benefit of being able to assess between-site discrepancies and consistencies.

Confirmatory trials constrain important treatment parameters such as dosage and frequency of interventions, whereas pragmatic psychedelic trials could exercise flexibility here, particularly given the nascent nature of the treatment model, where practitioners cannot confidently claim to know the best parameters for all individuals and indications. In the context of psychedelic therapy, what dosage, frequency-of-dosing, as well as frequency and nature of post-dosing psychotherapy sessions are optimal, and for which cases, are all key questions that may be best addressed via pragmatic research under a basket protocol – and/or via digital data collection. Upper limits on the number of dosing sessions and lower limits on the intervals between them may be set to reduce the risk of bad practice, but redosing in response to relapse and based on clinical judgement may be permitted, thereby reflecting the conditions of clinical practice post roll-out. Most modern trials of psychedelic therapy have employed just one or two fixed-dose treatment sessions for all participants within relatively small and homogeneous samples, not because of assumptions about best practice, but because of alignment with regulatory traditions and budget constraints.

This article argues that carefully designed pragmatic trials implemented under a basket protocol could offer a powerfully progressive model for advancing our understanding of the safety, effectiveness, mechanisms, impact, best-use and pitfalls of a promising but vulnerable new treatment model in psychiatry. Progressive policy changes would likely be needed to actualise the proposed approach – but these are already occurring. For such policy changes to occur, a vision of the societal value of improved mental health care, and how this can be safely and effectively achieved via psychedelic therapy, will need to be well communicated to the public and policy makers.

For the time being, DB-RCTs will continue to sway sceptical opinions and aid progress with regulators, who presently base pivotal licensing decisions on data derived from such trials. Our view, however, is that data derived from pragmatic trials may be able to teach us more (than DB-RCTs) about how best to deliver the treatment and how it could impact on the lives of a broad cross-section of people. To be clear, the argument here is for the complementary value of pragmatic trials, not for their superiority over DB-RCTs. At the same time, however, we do challenge, as others have previously, the hierarchical preeminence of DB-RCT-derived evidence (Tucker and Roth, 2006).

Exploration has special value early-on in a learning process; thus, it seems prudent in the context of psychedelic therapy that it be given consideration now, rather than further down the development path, when suboptimal parameters begin to undergo regulatory ‘lock-in’. Rectifying this matter now may help mitigate risks associated with a too hasty scale-up of access. To achieve this, however, buy-in from multiple stake holders will be needed, including the public, policy makers and those in between, e.g. scientists, clinicians and investors. The motivation for doing so is to protect the sustainability of psychedelic medicine.

There are signs that modern psychiatry is ripe for a radical ‘new’ treatment model, and psychedelic therapy offers a multi-level paradigm-challenge. Assumptions challenged by it include those pertaining to: theoretical frameworks in mental health, models of therapeutic action, selection of sufficiently sensitive and specific assessment scales, trial design and clinical practice, plus drug, economic and social policy. Here we propose that pragmatic trials, data registries and electronic data capture will aid advances in psychedelic medicine by catalysing our understanding of best practice, which includes, but is not limited to, identifying and mitigating risks.

Pragmatic trials, data registries and digital and biometric data collection can interface well with so-called ‘n = 1 trials’ (Lillie et al., 2011), where individuals and/or prescribing doctors assess, prospectively, the impact of introducing a time-limited intervention (e.g. the A-B-A design) in single-case studies. In contrast to large-scale observational cohort studies that allow for the modelling and prediction of response across wide demographics at the cost of experimental control (Haijen et al., 2018), single-subject designs assess the effectiveness of an intervention experimentally, thus representing a scientifically rigorous alternative to RCTs (Lobo et al., 2017). When participants serve as their own comparison (measured under different experimental conditions over time), confounding variables such as age, gender or socioeconomic status are automatically controlled for, thereby decreasing the number of participants required to determine the likelihood of a causal relationship between intervention and outcome, and ultimately, research costs (Charness and Fehr, 2015).

Single-case approaches also bear relevance to ‘citizen-science’ initiatives, in which individuals’ willingness to engage in the scientific process is harnessed. For example, individuals may be invited to increase the rigour of their ‘self-experimentation’ by, for example, completing assessments, wearing biometric sensor devices or even engaging in a self-blinding placebo-controlled protocol, as was done recently for psychedelic ‘microdosing’ (Szigeti et al., 2021).

As implied by some recent studies of ours (Haijen et al., 2018), there is appetite for citizen-science-type engagement among users of psychedelics. Specifically, we foresee value in the use of smartphone apps to collect data pertaining to psychedelic use in a convenient and efficient way (see https://www.mydelica.com). For example, the combination of single-case trial designs and remote digital assessments could enable the collection of scientifically rigorous efficacy data on self-medicative psychedelic use in small or difficult to access patient populations (Elliott, 2002), the potential utility of which is particularly salient considering the significant challenges that COVID-19 has posed on clinical and research psychiatry, including psychedelic trials (Kelly et al., 2020). Data from n = 1 experiments can be aggregated and analysed using Bayesian statistics (Zucker et al., 2010) and multi-level regression and post-stratification analyses (Spiegelhalter, 2019) to identify meaningful relationships within potentially rich datasets that could ultimately inform effective future care strategies. Idiographic high-frequency time-series data collected through such methods (e.g. daily smartphone-based assessments) could enable more ecologically valid and nuanced modelling of change than conventional study designs (Hofmann et al., 2020).

Zooming-out, the highlighted approach should not be interpreted as implying relaxed standards of screening or scientific rigour in psychedelic research. We are not, for example, advocating that researchers relax contraindication-based exclusion criteria intended to mitigate risks of adverse responses. Some might feel it premature to propose pragmatic trials for psychedelic therapy, as these are typically reserved for treatments that are already incorporated into clinical practice. However, we believe that it is right to begin such trials now, as policy changes are already afoot and could ‘get ahead of the data’, as occurred with cannabis, for example. There is presently insufficient data on which to recommend specific treatment parameters for specific populations and indications, as well as ‘no go’ criteria (at least for a standard treatment approach) at screening, and big data pools would likely change this. Indeed, large-scale datasets from naturalistic sampling could have considerable harm reduction potential, by helping identify those most and least suitable for psychedelic therapy.

Progressive policy changes on psychedelic medicine will likely have trickle down effects on research, innovation and investment in psychedelic medicine, particularly in the implicated geographical locations. Given the considerable cost-implications of a multi-site pragmatic research programme, health care payers (e.g. insurance companies) and/or industry buy-in would likely be required to fund it, and the relevant parties would need to be incentivised to do so. Digital data collection could lessen this burden, however, particularly if individual end-users (i.e. psychedelic users themselves) feel sufficiently incentivised to engage directly, e.g. via inputting data via a phone app (https://www.mydelica.com).

Mainstream, institutional-level funding has still not come into psychedelic science; philanthropy and now commercial investment have been its main drivers. Increasing demand for psychedelic therapy is poised to synergise with an upswell in initiatives to meet this, potentially jeopardising standards of safety and professionalism if corners are cut. In anticipation of and, to some extent, already witnessing the beginning of a ‘hype-cycle’, we believe that innovative, pragmatic and exploratory research can play a vital role, helping safeguard the development of a particularly promising, yet vulnerable, approach to mental health care.

Acknowledgments

The Centre for Psychedelic Research at Imperial College London was founded by donations from Ad Astra Chandaria Foundation, Nikean Foundation, Tim Ferriss, Alexander & Bohdana Tamas and Anton Bilton. RCH has been further supported by the Alex Mosley Charitable Trust. For a full list of our supporters, see our webpage: https://www.imperial.ac.uk/psychedelic-research-centre.

Authors’ note

David Erritzoe is also affiliated to CNWL-Imperial Psychopharmacology & Psychedelic Research (CIPPRes) Clinic, St. Charles Hospital, CNWL Mental Health NHS Foundation Trust, London, UK.

Declaration of conflicting interests

The author(s) declared the following potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: RCH is a scientific advisor to Entheon Biomedical, Synthesis, TRYP, COMPASS Pathways, Usona, Maya Health, Osmind and MyDecine. He is also Founder of the app MyDelica (https://www.mydelica.com). DE is a scientific advisor to Small Pharma and Field Trip. DJN is an advisor to COMPASS Pathways and AWAKN. All of the above are companies or organisations investing in psychedelic therapy. ACW has received research funding from the Multidisciplinary Association for Psychedelic Studies. AG and AM have no relevant conflicts of interest. JFR is Co-founder (and equity holder) of Psy Therapeutics LLC, an early discovery pipeline company focused on novel therapeutics discovery for psychiatric disorders and dementia (www.psythx.com), scientific advisor to Luminopia and scientific advisor to Terran Biosciences. DJN is editor of this journal and RCH, HK and DE are colleagues.

Funding

The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship and/or publication of this article: RCH is grateful to funders of the Centre for Psychedelic Research: https://www.imperial.ac.uk/psychedelic-research-centre/funding-partners/

ORCID iDs

Robin L Carhart-Harris https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6062-7150

Anne C Wagner https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6261-0133

Hannes Kettner https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9482-0998

David Erritzoe https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7022-6211

References

Alboni S, Van Dijk RM, Poggini S, et al. (2017) Fluoxetine effects on molecular, cellular and behavioral endophenotypes of depression are driven by the living environment. Mol Psychiatry 22: 552–561. Crossref. PubMed.

Andersen KAA, Carhart-Harris R, Nutt DJ, et al. (2020) Therapeutic effects of classic serotonergic psychedelics: A systematic review of modern-era clinical studies. Acta Psychiatr Scand 143: 101–118. Crossref. PubMed.

Armitage R (2021) Antidepressants, primary care, and adult mental health services in England during COVID-19. Lancet Psychiatry 8: e3. Crossref. PubMed.

Brainstorm C, Anttila V, Bulik-Sullivan B, et al. (2018) Analysis of shared heritability in common disorders of the brain. Science 360.

Brandessence Market Research Company Pvt Ltd (2019) Antidepressant drugs market size, share, current trends, opportunities, competitive analysis and forecast to 2019–2025. Available at: https://brandessenceresearch.biz

Brooks SJ, Stein DJ (2015) A systematic review of the neural bases of psychotherapy for anxiety and related disorders. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 17: 261–279. Crossref. PubMed. ISI.

Carhart-Harris RL (2018a) The entropic brain-revisited. Neuropharmacology 142: 167–178. Crossref. PubMed.

Carhart-Harris RL (2018b) Serotonin, psychedelics and psychiatry. World Psychiatry 17: 358–359. Crossref. PubMed.

Carhart-Harris RL, Bolstridge M, Rucker J, et al. (2016) Psilocybin with psychological support for treatment-resistant depression: An open-label feasibility study. Lancet Psychiatry 3: 619–627. Crossref. PubMed. ISI.

Carhart-Harris RL, Friston K (2019) REBUS and the anarchic brain: Toward a unified model of the brain action of psychedelics. Pharmacol Rev 71: 316–344. Crossref. PubMed.

Caspi A, Hariri AR, Holmes A, et al. (2010) Genetic sensitivity to the environment: The case of the serotonin transporter gene and its implications for studying complex diseases and traits. Am J Psychiatry 167: 509–527. Crossref. PubMed. ISI.

Charness G, Fehr E (2015) ECONOMICS. From the lab to the real world. Science 350: 512–513. Crossref. PubMed.

Cipriani A, Furukawa TA, Salanti G, et al. (2018) Comparative efficacy and acceptability of 21 antidepressant drugs for the acute treatment of adults with major depressive disorder: A systematic review and network meta-analysis. Focus 16: 420–429. Crossref. PubMed.

Clark DM (2011) Implementing NICE guidelines for the psychological treatment of depression and anxiety disorders: The IAPT experience. Int Rev Psychiatry 23: 318–327. Crossref. PubMed. ISI.

Copeland WE, Shanahan L, Costello EJ, et al. (2009) Childhood and adolescent psychiatric disorders as predictors of young adult disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 66: 764–772. Crossref. PubMed.

Davis AK, Barrett FS, May DG, et al. (2020) Effects of psilocybin-assisted therapy on major depressive disorder: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry. Epub ahead of print 4 November 2020. Crossref

Elliott R (2002) Hermeneutic single-case efficacy design. Psychother Res 12: 1–21. Crossref. PubMed. ISI.

Engel GL (1977) The need for a new medical model: A challenge for biomedicine. Science 196: 129–136. Crossref. PubMed. ISI.

Flint J, Cuijpers P, Horder J, et al. (2015) Is there an excess of significant findings in published studies of psychotherapy for depression? Psychol Med 45: 439–446. Crossref. PubMed. ISI.

Forstmann M, Yudkin DA, Prosser AM, et al. (2020) Transformative experience and social connectedness mediate the mood-enhancing effects of psychedelic use in naturalistic settings. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 117: 2338–2346. Crossref. PubMed.

Freeman AM, Tribe RH, Stott JCH, et al. (2019) Open dialogue: A review of the evidence. Psychiatr Serv 70: 46–59. Crossref. PubMed.

Global Wellness Institute (2019) 2018 global wellness economy monitor. Available at: https://globalwellnessinstitute.org/industry-research/2018-global-wellness-economy-monitor/

Griffiths RR, Johnson MW, Carducci MA, et al. (2016) Psilocybin produces substantial and sustained decreases in depression and anxiety in patients with life-threatening cancer: A randomized double-blind trial. J Psychopharmacol 30: 1181–1197. Crossref. PubMed. ISI.

Griffiths RR, Johnson MW, Richards WA, et al. (2018) Psilocybin-occasioned mystical-type experience in combination with meditation and other spiritual practices produces enduring positive changes in psychological functioning and in trait measures of prosocial attitudes and behaviors. J Psychopharmacol 32: 49–69. Crossref. PubMed. ISI.

Haijen EC, Kaelen M, Roseman L, et al. (2018) Predicting responses to psychedelics: A prospective study. Front Pharmacol 9: 897. Crossref. PubMed.

Harvey JA (2003) Role of the serotonin 5-HT(2A) receptor in learning. Learn Mem 10: 355–362. Crossref. PubMed. ISI.

Hayes S (2019) A Liberated Mind: The Essential Guide to ACT. Vermilion. London: Ebury Publishing.

Hendricks PS, Thorne CB, Clark CB, et al. (2015) Classic psychedelic use is associated with reduced psychological distress and suicidality in the United States adult population. J Psychopharmacol 29: 280–288. Crossref. PubMed. ISI.

Hofmann SG, Curtiss JE, Hayes SC (2020) Beyond linear mediation: Toward a dynamic network approach to study treatment processes. Clin Psychol Rev 76: 101824. Crossref. PubMed.

Inkster B, O’Brien R, Selby E, et al. (2020) Digital health management during and beyond the COVID-19 pandemic: Opportunities, barriers, and recommendations. JMIR Ment Health 7: e19246. Crossref. PubMed.

Insel TR, Cuthbert BN (2015) Brain disorders? precisely. Science 348: 499–500. Crossref. PubMed. ISI.

James SL, Abate D, Abate KH, et al. (2018) Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 354 diseases and injuries for 195 countries and territories, 1990–2017: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2017. Lancet 392: 1789–1858. Crossref. PubMed.

Johnson MW, Garcia-Romeu A, Cosimano MP, et al. (2014) Pilot study of the 5-HT2AR agonist psilocybin in the treatment of tobacco addiction. J Psychopharmacol 28: 983–992. Crossref. PubMed. ISI.

Kelly JR, Crockett MT, Alexander L, et al. (2020) Psychedelic science in post-COVID-19 psychiatry. Ir J Psychol Med. Epub ahead of print 19 August 2020. Crossref

Leff J, Williams G, Huckvale M, et al. (2014) Avatar therapy for persecutory auditory hallucinations: What is it and how does it work? Psychosis 6: 166–176. Crossref. PubMed.

Lewis G, Duffy L, Ades A, et al. (2019) The clinical effectiveness of sertraline in primary care and the role of depression severity and duration (PANDA): A pragmatic, double-blind, placebo-controlled randomised trial. Lancet Psychiatry 6: 903–914. Crossref. PubMed.

Lillie EO, Patay B, Diamant J, et al. (2011) The n-of-1 clinical trial: The ultimate strategy for individualizing medicine? Per Med 8: 161–173. Crossref. PubMed. ISI.

Lobo MA, Moeyaert M, Baraldi Cunha A, et al. (2017) Single-case design, analysis, and quality assessment for intervention research. J Neurol Phys Ther 41: 187–197. Crossref. PubMed.

Ly C, Greb AC, Cameron LP, et al. (2018) Psychedelics promote structural and functional neural plasticity. Cell Rep 23: 3170–3182. Crossref. PubMed.

Mahase E (2021) Covid-19: Children’s mental health services in England are “nowhere near sufficient,” says commissioner. BMJ 372: n258. Crossref. PubMed.

Massabki I, Abi-Jaoude E (2021) Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor ‘discontinuation syndrome’ or withdrawal. Br J Psychiatry 218: 168–171. Crossref. PubMed.

Moller NP, Ryans G, Rollings J, et al. (2019) The 2018 UK NHS digital annual report on the improving access to psychological therapies programme: A brief commentary. BMC Psychiatry 19: 252. Crossref. PubMed.

Moreno FA, Wiegand CB, Taitano EK, et al. (2006) Safety, tolerability, and efficacy of psilocybin in 9 patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 67: 1735–1740. Crossref. PubMed. ISI.

Nichols DE (2017) Chemistry and structure–activity relationships of psychedelics. In: Halberstadt AL, Vollenweider FX, Nichols DE (eds) Behavioral Neurobiology of Psychedelic Drugs. Berlin: Springer, pp.1–43. Crossref.

Nutt D, Carhart-Harris R (2021) The current status of psychedelics in psychiatry. JAMA Psychiatry 78: 121–122. Crossref. PubMed.

Nutt D, Erritzoe D, Carhart-Harris R (2020) Psychedelic psychiatry’s brave new world. Cell 181: 24–28. Crossref. PubMed.

Ona G, Kohek M, Massaguer T, et al. (2019) Ayahuasca and public health: Health status, psychosocial well-being, lifestyle, and coping strategies in a large sample of ritual ayahuasca users. J Psychoactive Drugs 51: 135–145. Crossref. PubMed.

Parmar MK, Sydes MR, Cafferty FH, et al. (2017) Testing many treatments within a single protocol over 10 years at MRC Clinical Trials Unit at UCL: Multi-arm, multi-stage platform, umbrella and basket protocols. Clin Trials 14: 451–461. Crossref. PubMed. ISI.

Patton GC, Sawyer SM, Santelli JS, et al. (2016) Our future: A Lancet commission on adolescent health and wellbeing. Lancet 387: 2423–2478. Crossref. PubMed. ISI.

Purgato M, Barbui C, Stroup S, et al. (2015) Pragmatic design in randomized controlled trials. Psychol Med 45: 225–230. Crossref. PubMed.

Renelli M, Fletcher J, Tupper KW, et al. (2020) An exploratory study of experiences with conventional eating disorder treatment and ceremonial ayahuasca for the healing of eating disorders. Eat Weight Disord 25: 437–444. Crossref. PubMed.

Richards D, Duffy D, Blackburn B, et al. (2018) Digital IAPT: The effectiveness & cost-effectiveness of internet-delivered interventions for depression and anxiety disorders in the Improving Access to Psychological Therapies programme: Study protocol for a randomised control trial. BMC Psychiatry 18: 59. Crossref. PubMed.

Ross S, Bossis A, Guss J, et al. (2016) Rapid and sustained symptom reduction following psilocybin treatment for anxiety and depression in patients with life-threatening cancer: A randomized controlled trial. J Psychopharmacol 30: 1165–1180. Crossref. PubMed. ISI.

Rucker JJ, Iliff J, Nutt DJ (2018) Psychiatry & the psychedelic drugs. Past, present & future. Neuropharmacology 142: 200–218. Crossref. PubMed.

Segal ZV, Teasdale J (2018) Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy for Depression. New York: Guilford Publications.

Spiegelhalter D (2019) The Art of Statistics: Learning from Data. London: Penguin UK.

Spriggs MJ, Kettner H, Carhart-Harris RL (2020) Positive effects of psychedelics on depression and wellbeing scores in individuals reporting an eating disorder. Eat Weight Disord. Epub ahead of print 8 September 2020. Crossref PubMed.

Szigeti B, Kartner L, Blemings A, et al. (2021) Self-blinding citizen science to explore psychedelic microdosing. Elite 10: e62878.

Trautmann S, Rehm J, Wittchen HU (2016) The economic costs of mental disorders: Do our societies react appropriately to the burden of mental disorders? EMBO Rep 17: 1245–1249. Crossref. PubMed.

Tucker JA, Roth DL (2006) Extending the evidence hierarchy to enhance evidence-based practice for substance use disorders. Addiction 101: 918–932. Crossref. PubMed. ISI.

Twenge JM, Cooper AB, Joiner TE, et al. (2019) Age, period, and cohort trends in mood disorder indicators and suicide-related outcomes in a nationally representative dataset, 2005–2017. J Abnorm Psychol 128: 185–199. Crossref. PubMed.

Varela FJ (1996) Neurophenomenology: A methodological remedy for the hard problem. J Conscious Stud 3: 330–349.

Varese F, Smeets F, Drukker M, et al. (2012) Childhood adversities increase the risk of psychosis: A meta-analysis of patient-control, prospective-and cross-sectional cohort studies. Schizophr Bull 38: 661–671. Crossref. PubMed. ISI.

Wade DT, Halligan PW (2017) The Biopsychosocial Model of Illness: A Model Whose Time Has Come. London: SAGE Publications.

Whelan A, Johnson MI (2018) Lysergic acid diethylamide and psilocybin for the management of patients with persistent pain: A potential role? Pain Manag 8: 217–229. Crossref. PubMed.

Xing L, Kalebic N, Namba T, et al. (2020) Serotonin receptor 2A activation promotes evolutionarily relevant basal progenitor proliferation in the developing neocortex. Neuron 108: 1113–1129.e6. Crossref. PubMed.

Zucker DR, Ruthazer R, Schmid CH (2010) Individual (N-of-1) trials can be combined to give population comparative treatment effect estimates: Methodologic considerations. J Clin Epidemiol 63: 1312–1323. Crossref. PubMed. ISI.